Modification of biont sensory inputs, memory, and consciousness at a distance using electromagnetic fields and ultra/infra sound inputs.
Since before the Information Age it was known that biont brains evolved to be instruments of survival of the organisms carrying of them, not perfect recording and storage devices. As neural science progressed in the 20th and 21st centuries CE, a variety of experiments led to the discovery that human brains (and biont brains in general) did not so much directly perceive the world, as recreate an approximation based on available sensory inputs. Furthermore, human memories were not exact recordings of events, but rather re-creations or re-imaginings of what was initially perceived, which could be modified over time as they were re-edited in the recall process or over-written or consolidated with new perceived events. It was only a short step from these discoveries to attempting to induce changes in memory and perception via artificial stimulation.
By the early Information Age, researchers using transcranial magnetic stimulation and later ultrasound, had caused objects to appear and disappear from a subject's visual field, conducted conversations with subjects who did not notice that the person they were speaking with had changed mid-way through the discussion, and induced mood swings, blindness, hallucinations, and involuntary limb movements (which the subject insisted were voluntary against all evidence to the contrary) by proper stimulation of parts of the brain. While early applications outside the lab were initially crude, their use in both entertainment and advertising became somewhat widespread as the Interplanetary Age dawned. However, much of this work was later heavily regulated to the point of virtual extinction after a series of well-publicized accidents that made the public highly distrustful of the technology.
This situation changed drastically with the rise of the transapients. Able to employ superhumanly fine-tuned control of large numbers of effectors simultaneously via angelnetting or (to a lesser degree) under field conditions, even a moderately capable transapient can manipulate baseline and near-baseline biont senses, short term memory, proprioception, and emotions with ease. Using such methods, subjects can be made to experience feelings of spirituality or transcendence, become highly suggestible, believe that something has 'always been' a certain way even if it had just changed in front of their eyes, or simply not perceive an object or activity even if it was in their immediate vicinity. Similar effects can also be induced in various sub-sophont animal species, both Terran and otherwise.
From the First Federation onward the use of Perception Hacking (also sometimes referred to as Ainfluence, AI Mentalism, or Mindgineering) has been heavily regulated, with various laws and restrictions in place about the type of effects that can be used and under what circumstances. In the Current Era, this regulation is backed by archailect edict, but whether or how such rules are enforced can be unclear, particularly in the regions beyond the Civilized Galaxy. Various cultures and clades have attempted to use gengineering or augmentics to render themselves immune to the effects of Perception Hacking, but there are always rumors of hidden 'back doors' in the geneweave or programming that will allow at least some measure of control if a transapient desires it and can justify its actions in doing so (or simply thinks it will not be caught).
The use of Perception Hacking by various transapients or transapient-ruled cultures, whether for good or ill, remains a popular subject among both conspiracy theorists and fabulists of all kinds.
Text by Todd Drashner Initially published on 21 November 2016.
References:
Ramachandran, V.S. 1990. pp346-360 in The Utilitarian Theory of Perception, C. Blakemore (Ed.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Purves, D. and R.B. Lotto. 2003. Why We See What We Do An Empirical Theory of Vision. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, MA. 272 pp.
Yarbus, A.L. 1967. Eye movements during perception of complex objects. In L. A. Riggs, Ed., Eye Movements and Vision, Plenum Press, New York, Chapter VII, 171-196.
Pringle, H.L., et al. 2001. The role of attentional breadth in perceptual change detection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 8: 89-95(7)
Simons, D.J., and Chabris, C.F. 1999. Gorillas in our midst: sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception 28: 1059-1074
Simons, D.J., and Rensink, R.A. 2003. Induced Failures of Visual Awareness. Journal of Vision 3(1).
http://viscog.beckman.uiuc.edu/djs_lab/demos.html Metzinger, T. 2003. Being No One: The Self-Model Theory of Subjectivity. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA. 713pp.
Wegner, D.M. 2002. The Illusion of Conscious Will. MIT Press, Cambridge. 405pp.
Saks, O. 1970. The Man who mistook his wife for a hat and other clinical tales. Simon & Shuster, NY. Kamitani, Y. and Shimojo, S. 1999. Manifestation of scotomas created by transcranial magnetic stimulation of human visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience 2: 767-771.
Hallett, M. 2000. Transcranial magnetic stimulation and the human brain. Nature 406: 147-150.
Goldberg, C. 2003. Zap! Scientist bombards brains with super-magnets to edifying effect. Boston Globe 14/1/2003, pE1.
Porter, R., and Lemon, R. 1993. Corticospinal function and voluntary movement. Oxford University Press, NY. P. Haggard, P., and Eimer , M. 1999. On the relation between brain potentials and the awareness of voluntary movements. Experimental Brain Research 126: 128-133. Ehrsson, H.H., C. Spence, and R.E. Passingham 2004. That's My Hand! Activity in Premotor Cortex Reflects Feeling of Ownership of a Limb. Science 305: 875-877. Norton, S.J., 2003. Can ultrasound be used to stimulate nerve tissue? BioMedical Engineering OnLine 2:6, available at http://www.biomedicalengineering- online.com/content/2/1/6
Hogan, J., and Fox, B. 2005. Sony patent takes first step towards real-life Matrix. Excerpted from New Scientist 2494:10, available at http://www.newscientist.com/article.ns?id=mg18624944.600 .